
So what’s my problem with optogenetics? Actually, I have several problems. First is the gross oversell. For optogenetics to become an effective method for treating mental illnesses, you need specific knowledge about the illnesses’ neural underpinnings. You must know which neurons or neural circuits are overactive or underactive or otherwise abnormal.
But we lack such knowledge about depression, schizophrenia, bipolar disorder or any other major mental illness. As the director of the National Institute of Mental Health recently acknowledged, decades of research have not turned up any clear-cut physiological—that is, neural, genetic or chemical–correlates of the major mental illnesses. How can a brain-manipulation technique alleviate mental illness if we don’t know what to manipulate?
[from Why “Optogenetic” Methods for Manipulating Brains Don’t Light Me Up* | Cross-Check, Scientific American Blog Network.]
Great, concise phrasing. Unfortunately it overlooks one of the past decade’s most significant lines of work in depression. This is the work of Helen Mayberg, which I’ve written about in a long feature in the New York Times Magazine 7 years ago; in a follow-up in Scientific American; and a post on optogenetics here. (You’re encouraged to read and judge for yourself whether I overhype the work.)
Mayberg’s work presents exactly what Horgan says is absent: The identification of a specific target for the use of things like optogenetics. In a line of work 25 years and running now, Mayberg first painstakingly identified a specific brain region, known as Area 25 (labeled ACg in the center figure above), that seems to run amok in depression; and then, by quieting Area 25 in experimental surgeries that implanted deep brain stimulation (DBS) devices in it, achieved two rounds of remarkable results alleviating depression in a majority of horribly depressed patients. She continues to get promising results with this experimental technique, and large double-blind trials are in progress.

Neurologist Helen Mayberg’s hand-drawn diagram (in my notebook one day) of Area 25 connectivity involved in depression. Drawing and labels are Mayberg’s; handwritten notes above and below are mine.
Mayberg is quick to lodge two caveats that I think Horgan would be quick to lodge: First, that before we can consider this a viable treatment, the double-blind trials underway need to replicate her finding. Second, that the intrusiveness and expense of deep brain stimulation surgery, and perhaps of optogenetics, make those approaches to this area less than ideal, and we must hope to find another way to quiet this area if indeed the trials prove its viability.
I’m not saying this is a treatment ready for prime time, despite its great promise. I am saying that given these results, it’s misleading — no, as much as I like Horgan, I must say it’s just wrong to declare that no one has yet identified any neurological correlates or targets for tools such as optogenetics or DBS. If Horgan wants to argue that by “clear-cut” he means “absolutely proven,” then … he needs to change “clear-cut” to “absolutely proven.”
But if we’re with Webster in defining “clear-cut” as “sharply outlined” or “distinct,” then I hope Horgan will agree that we can meet that definition by offering a sharply outlined and distinct pea-sized brain area whose targeting relieves crippling depression in half to two-thirds of catastrophically, otherwise incurably depressed patients.
Please proceed.
Note on top image: For some reason it wouldn’t let me add a caption, so here it is: Brain areas affected by four historical psychosurgical depression treatments. Area 25, aka the anterior cingulate, is identified as ACg in the center figure.
You do yourself a disservice by continuing to champion the work of Mayberg. Your readers should check out critiques of Mayberg by journalist Alison Bass. http://alison-bass.com/blog/2011/06/is-the-credibility-of-emory-neurologist-helen-mayberg-in-question-you-judge/
John, you do yourself a disservice by citing Bass’s work. I have categorically refuted Bass’s post, first in the comments section of the posts where she made her false accusations, and then, when those corrections in her comments never passed Bass’s spam or approval filters, in a Posterous post of my own. Since Posterous closed this summer, I have today moved that corrective to this Neuron Culture post, which I backdated for historical accuracy. It completely refutes Bass’s charges against Mayberg. It doesn’t do much for your credibility to cite Bass’s delusional accusations as a rear-guard attack on my correction of your own facts.
Bass also falsely accused me of changing something on Mayberg’s Wikipedia page. She retracted that accusation later, probably for fear of lawsuit — but removed my comment asking her to do so and has never corrected any of the errors I pointed out, nor has she ever published any of my comments at her site pointing them out. All this stemming from Bass’s own false accusations of lack of transparency.
Alison Bass, Your Facts on Helen Mayberg Are Wrong, Wrong, and Wrong
John, Would you care to respond in any way to the actual factual errors I pointed out: That your claim of no targets was inaccurate? It’s not just I who think Mayberg’s work is significant in that aspect, and that its existence refutes your point. The director of NIMH, Tom Insel, thinks so too. He has said many times that he considers Mayberg’s work and findings about Area 25 to be among the most significant lines of work in mental health over the past decade. They represent the sort of brain-based investigations and findings that he hopes to pursue in a research program made independent of the DSM. If you don’t believe that, ask him.